We can work on Memory Errors In Real Life

Laypersons are often unaware of the extent to which memory errors (including memory of events that did not occur and memory distortions) are commonplace and widespread. The purpose of this assignment is to make you think of not only memory errors in real life, but also the reasons behind their occurrence. Consider the steps listed below:
Read the “Adaptive Constructive Processes and the Future of Memory” and “A Range of Memory Possibilities: The Challenge of the false Memory Debate for Clinicians and Researchers” articles, which offer an overview of memory errors and their origins.
Keep in mind that your textbook is another valuable source of information regarding memory errors.
Select a case mentioned in a news article where memory errors have been known or can be assumed to have occurred (e.g., eyewitness testimony, instances of recovered memories of traumatic experiences, etc.).
Describe the main features of the errors, their consequences, and their possible sources (i.e., what are the features of human memory that make the errors possible?).
Engage your critical thinking skills:
Explain what cognitive psychology suggests that people do to decrease the occurrence of the memory errors that you discuss in your paper. If these errors cannot be avoided, what does cognitive psychology suggest that people do to prevent the often damaging consequences of such errors?
Note that statements regarding the inaccuracy of human memory, and strategies to decrease the occurrence of errors and their consequences are to be supported by scientific evidence collected by experts. Thus, review the literature on memory errors and use at least two peer-reviewed articles that can answer the questions above.
Must begin with an introduction to the selected topic in which you define all concepts that will be discussed in the paper.
Include a brief description of the memory errors featured in the selected news article as well as discussing potential sources and known or likely consequences according to the guidelines described above.
Include a conclusion expressing your thoughts about how these memory errors can be prevented or reduced, and the extent to which their often dreadful consequences can be minimized. Keep in mind that although your opinion is valuable, it needs to be supported by scientific evidence.

7.1 Everyday Memory
When we tell people that we teach psychology, this information elicits knowing glances or furrowed brows, or, worse yet, comments like, “Ohhhh…going to analyze me, huh?” We quickly inform these new acquaintances that no, we don’t really care about their problems (in a clinical sense, at least); we do memory research. This triggers an entirely different (and at times, equally frustrating) set of questions and comments, such as, “Why do I have such a terrible memory for my childhood?” or “I’ll never forget what I was doing when I heard that Princess Diana was killed” or “Why do I remember things differently than my spouse?” Why should we, as memory researchers, find these questions about memory frustrating? Because, until fairly recently, we haven’t had very good answers to these questions. These questions are partially answered by material discussed in Chapter 6. But there is something unique about these questions, something that goes unaddressed by the material presented about memory to this point. These questions refer to one’s personal experience and use of memory in an everyday context. As such, they aren’t fully addressed by much of the memory research discussed thus far.
Neisser’s Challenge: Ecological Validity and Memory Research
As you’ve no doubt noticed, cognitive psychology research is quite elegant: every confounding variable is anticipated and controlled, and each aspect of responding is carefully and precisely measured. To put it in terms of a concept introduced in Chapter 1, the internal validity of cognitive research is nothing short of impressive. Cognitive psychology no doubt owes behaviorism a debt of gratitude on this score; behaviorist challenges to the notion of scientifically analyzing mental processes (as defined by structuralists and functionalists) meant that any new approach to the study of cognition would have to live up to extremely high methodological standards. Cognitive psychology has more than answered the challenge.
But while saluting the incredible progress cognitive psychology has made in establishing an empirical base, many would say that it has come at a cost. Often, the ecological validity (also termed external validity) of cognitive psychology research has fallen well short of impressive. There exists a natural tension between the internal validity and the ecological validity of any research enterprise. You trade off one to get more of the other; more controlled is less natural, and vice versa. Take memory, for example; a great deal of the research in the first three decades of experimental work on memory fit a pretty standard pattern. Word lists carefully constructed to control for the effects of various extraneous variables were presented under precisely controlled conditions, and participant memory was tested with a number of standard laboratory memory tasks, like free recall or recognition. But one might argue that the number of words you can recall from a list of 48 concrete and abstract words presented for four seconds each doesn’t tell you that much about why you can’t remember events from your life that occurred before age 3. The emphasis in memory research (and cognition research in general) has always been on internal validity, often at the expense of ecological validity.
In an important address to cognitive psychologists in 1977, Ulric Neisser delivered a blistering critique of the memory research that had accumulated in the first quarter-century of cognition research, a critique that was directed at the emphasis on internal validity. According to Neisser (1978), “If X is an interesting or socially significant aspect of memory, then psychologists have hardly ever studied X” (p. 4). These were fighting words, to say the least. Neisser was basically saying that the first 30 years of memory research had been boring and trivial. Neisser went even further, claiming that although firm empirical generalizations about memory had indeed been made, most of these generalizations “are so obvious that every 10-year-old child knows them anyway” (p. 4). Extremely harsh words, perhaps, but many cognitive psychologists took them to heart. Since Neisser’s address, there has been a veritable explosion of research on what might be considered “everyday” memory.
The explosion is evident in the number of citations found in the psychology research database PsychINFO, using the search terms “everyday memory” and “autobiographical memory.” Up until 1986, only a few dozen psychology articles were directed at issues of everyday memory. Since then, there have been over 2500 investigations of these phenomena. Clearly, the investigation of how one remembers the events of their lives has become a central topic within the study of cognition.
Everyday Memory Research: Bankrupt?
Not everyone jumped on Neisser’s bandwagon. In an influential counter-critique, Banaji and Crowder (1989) decried what they termed the “bankruptcy of everyday memory,” objecting strongly to most of Neisser’s claims. They drew an analogy between the psychologist who conducts well-controlled basic laboratory research on memory and the chemist who does controlled experimentation on the properties of yeast in order to establish why bread dough rises. In their view, the precisely controlled experimentation of the chemist is a more sensible approach and is more likely to yield meaningful results than “loitering in professional bakeries and taking careful notes” (p. 1187). They add that memory psychologists should not be embarrassed or frustrated when faced with questions that can’t really be answered by basic research. “What other science,” they ask, “has established that its students should decide on the importance of questions by checking first with Aunt Martha?” (p. 1187). Banaji and Crowder argue that in everyday memory contexts (such as attempting to remember events from one’s life), the uncontrolled factors are so numerous that generalizability of the results is limited, not increased. Banaji and Crowder assert that the emphasis on internal validity in investigations of memory is entirely appropriate and is likely to be the road to truly generalizable principles of memory function.
Striking a Middle Ground.
Following Banaji and Crowder’s (1989) critique, a number of researchers came to the defense of everyday memory research (e.g., Conway, 1991) and/or emphasized the value of both laboratory and everyday approaches (e.g., Loftus & Ketcham, 1991; Tulving, 1991). Tulving (1991) makes the important point that memory research is not a “zero-sum game” (i.e., someone must win, and someone must lose); forsaking everyday memory research for a basic laboratory approach, or vice versa, would be “throwing the baby out with the bath water.” Both approaches can be quite valid and generalizable, and both approaches should be employed to discover the principles of memory function.
Since the opening salvos of the everyday memory debate, the dust has settled, and to no one’s surprise, both laboratory and everyday approaches to the study of memory are still standing. And both approaches are probably the richer for the exchange. Laboratory psychologists are more sensitive to issues of ecological validity, and everyday memory researchers are more sensitive to issues of precision and control (i.e., internal validity). In this chapter, we’ll review some of the discoveries that have been made by researchers investigating issues of everyday memory, so you’ll be well informed in your conversations with “Aunt Martha.”

Sample Solution

emorable experience to guests that they would not encounter somewhere else in this way tempting and affecting the potential guest to pick their goal over another. Be that as it may, goal marking can’t occur without the production of a goal picture. A huge scope of existing writing concurs that a goals picture is a visual portrayal of the goal in the traveler’s psyche. (Josiassen et al., 2015) An unmistakable conviction among those that have considered goal advertising is that a goals picture, if positive, will build up the probability of a person to visit a specific goal yet will likewise contribute emphatically to their experience and the degree to which they are fulfilled. (Stylises et al., 2017). As (Aaker 1991) states that a brand picture is an assortment of the considerable number of affiliations that connection a shopper’s memory to a brand. Accordingly, it is of outrageous significance to a goal to have a positive picture as it as it increases the value of the brand or for this situation a goal. A brand picture is said to include an incentive in different manners. It empowers the shopper to acquire data about the brand. It takes into account separation of the brands items and impacts the items advertise position. A brand picture likewise gives the client motivations to buy that brands items and furthermore cultivates a positive feeling inside the purchaser to the brand (Aaker 1994). While relating this hypothesis to a goal, it is said that the picture is the enlightening convictions one has about a nation yet in addition the view and the notoriety of a goal that an individual has of the spot. It is said that a goals picture according to visitors is comprised of three unmistakable segments. These are psychological, full of feeling and conative (Pike and Ryan 2004). The subjective picture is the segment that identifies with the traveler comprehension and assessment of the goals traits (Crompton, 1979) A goals characteristics can be different components, for example, the contribution of the goal as far as settlement, the view and setting of the goal and even the neighborliness of local people. In a failte Ireland report made in 2018 recording the statistical data points identifying with the travel industry in Ireland in 2017 coming up next was distinguished in connection to qualities esteemed most importa>

Is this question part of your assignment?

Place order